Promote the need for sincere information for a democracy based on trust
About us
|
OUR PUBLICATIONS
|
Nos activités
|
panorama
Soutenir la fondation
Fondation Descartes

The Fondation Descartes is a citizen-based, non-partisan, and independent European foundation dedicated to information-based issues.

Operation and governance

La Fondation Descartes est animée par trois organes : un Conseil d’Administration présidé par Jean-Philippe Hecketsweiler ; un Conseil Scientifique encadré par Gérald Bronner et d’une équipe permanente, dirigée par Laurent Cordonier, Directeur de la recherche.

Contact us

Vous souhaitez contacter l’équipe permanente de la Fondation Descartes ?

Our reports

La Fondation Descartes est une initiative citoyenne, apartisane, indépendante et européenne dédiée aux enjeux de l’information, de la désinformation et du débat public dans une société démocratique.

Thematic overviews

La Fondation Descartes est animée par trois organes : un Conseil d’Administration présidé par Jean-Philippe Hecketsweiler ; un Conseil Scientifique encadré par Gérald Bronner et d’une équipe permanente, dirigée par Laurent Cordonier, Directeur de la recherche.

Experts' blog

Vous souhaitez contacter l’équipe permanente de la Fondation Descartes ?

Events

The Descartes Foundation lists significant events, in France and internationally, particularly related to the topics of disinformation, interference and media literacy.

Our partnerships

La Fondation Descartes est animée par trois organes : un Conseil d’Administration présidé par Jean-Philippe Hecketsweiler ; un Conseil Scientifique encadré par Gérald Bronner et d’une équipe permanente, dirigée par Laurent Cordonier, Directeur de la recherche.

Our podcasts

Selection of 'must-listen' podcasts on the Descartes Foundation's fields of research, in French, English or German.

Actors

The Descartes Foundation offers you a cartography of the main actors involved in researching on the quality of information, or in fighting against disinformation, in France and throughout the world.

Initiatives

Fact checkers, web extensions, journalistic standards... The Fondation Descartes offers you a map of initiatives in France and around the world involved in asserting the quality of information or in fighting against disinformation.

Our references and resources

The Fondation Descartes' experts select and comment on key publications on disinformation, trust in the media and the Foundation's other research topics. This data base is available online to members of the Foundation and in the Foundation's documentation centre.

Inscrivez-vous à la newsletter

Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 American Election

Hunt Allcott and Matthew Gentzkow
01/04/2020

This article, published in 2017, is now one of the most cited references in scientific literature on fake news. It was one of the first to investigate this phenomenon. We here at Fondation Descartes have created a detailed summary for you. Readers in a hurry can go straight to the conclusion.

Aurélien Brest
Chargé d'études en psychologie cognitive à la Fondation Descartes

Background

Many fake news stories were circulating at the time of the 2016 election in the United States. With most criticising the Democrats or defending the Republicans, some commentators publicly raised concerns that Donald Trump's election was influenced by fake news.

Although this article does not provide a clear answer to that question, it does provide interesting data to better understand the fake news phenomenon in context of the 2016 presidential election.

Theoretical aspects

The article defines fake news as news articles that are intentionally false and could mislead readers. It chooses to include satirical and parody news stories that circulate on the web. The authors justify this choice by referring to the fake news story stating that the Pope had endorsed Trump's candidacy in the election. The story was meant to be a humorous prank, but it was widely shared, and it is possible that many individuals truly believed it.

The article then puts forward an economic model of fake news. The media organisations that produce fake news may be encouraged to do so because consumers sometimes tend to prefer news stories that align with their beliefs. So, a news story, even if it is fake, has a higher chance of being read and believed if it aligns with the expectations of an individual. Certain media organisations may therefore opt for fake news stories because they match the public's expectations and win their loyalty. In a political context, the authors envisage that individuals expect positive news stories about their candidate, no matter whether they are necessarily true or not. This hypothesis was tested in the study. However, publishing fake news can, over time, destroy a news outlet's reputation, and that is where the economics of social media comes in. Social networks are a favourable environment for fake news to spread. Publishing content on social media has virtually zero cost, so anybody can enter the market and produce fake news that then mixes with the rest of news media. This favours short-term strategies, and so, long-term reputation is not a factor in this kind of economy. Moreover, as news on social media is formatted to be attractive and simplified, fake news can slip in easily. This hypothesis was also tested in the study. More broadly, the article aims to determine in what proportion American voters were exposed to fake news and if they believed them.

Method:

To test these two hypotheses, the authors used a questionnaire on 28 November 2016 (two weeks after Donald Trump's election). As the sample group of participants (1200) was not representative of American society, the authors reweighted their sample to get as close as possible. As well as demographic, social and political questions for the survey participants, the questionnaire included 15 headlines in Facebook format. For each headline, participants were asked to respond if a) they recalled seeing it online; b) if they believed it to be true or false at the time.

These 15 headlines were randomly showed to participants out of a pool of 30 headlines selected by the authors. Half of the 30 headlines were pro-Clinton and the other half were pro-Trump. Among these 30 headlines, 20 were fake. As the participants were asked to recall if they had already seen the headlines being shown to them, the authors used a classic psychological test to make sure that participants were not inventing their memories. Among the 20 fake news headlines that could be randomly shown to participants, half were made up entirely for the study (placebos). So, if participants responded overwhelmingly that they recalled these placebos, the results for the real fake news would need to be interpreted with caution, as the participants’ memories cannot be trusted.

Results

Exposure to fake news. On average, 15% of participants responded that they recalled being exposed to the real fake news. However, around 14% of participants indicated that they recalled the placebos. 8% responded that they believed the fake news and 8% the placebos. This result is problematic insofar as the placebos were nearly just as recognised as the real fake news. So, it seems difficult to trust participants’ memories as they remember (on average) the placebos just as much as the real fake news. The authors considered that the small difference observed between the rate of recall for fake news and placebos is still able to be extrapolated to give how many fake news headlines Americans could have been exposed to, on average. By linking this difference between real fake news and placebos to the number of times that the real fake news stories used in the study were shared on Facebook, the authors estimate that an American could, on average, have seen and truly recall one in ten of the real fake news stories selected. Given the method used by the authors, this number should be taken with caution.

Prevalence of fake news on social media. According to the authors’ theoretical model, fake news is most often found on social media. To confirm this, they collected a sample of more than 600 websites that produce fake news and, using the website Alexa, they were able to determine that social media represented 41.8% of traffic on these sites. So, it seems that social media is highly favourable for the sharing of fake news. Furthermore, pro-Trump fake news stories were nearly 4 times more common on these sites than pro-Clinton ones. Though it is impossible to determine if pro-Trump fake news had an influence on the result of the American election, it is still important to underline that during this election, all factors being equal, voters were 8 times more likely to be exposed to a pro-Trump disinformation campaign.

Factors behind belief in fake news. The authors then created a statistical model to determine what might predict an individual mistaking a fake news story for a true news story. The factors identified by the authors are being a Republican (though this predictor is not very robust, according to the model), using social media as your most important source of news, being younger (the younger we are, the more likely we are to believe fake news according to the model) and being comparatively less educated than other participants. Finally, the authors were able to determine that pro-Trump fake news was more likely to be believed by Republicans and, vice versa, pro-Clinton news more believed by Democrats. The authors’ hypothesis was therefore confirmed - individuals tend to be more credulous when news stories align with their political convictions. It is important to note that this result held true for all headlines (true and false), not for fake news. The model does not state if the result can be explained by individuals being more likely to believe true news stories or because they are more credulous of fake news stories (or both).

Les facteurs de la croyance aux fake news. Les auteurs ont ensuite construit un modèle statistique pour déterminer ce qui permet de prédire qu'un individu se trompe en prenant une fake news pour une information vraie. Les facteurs identifiés par les auteurs sont le fait d'être partisan républicain (mais ce prédicteur est peu fiable selon le modèle), le fait d'utiliser les réseaux sociaux comme première source d'information, le fait d'être jeune (plus on est jeune et plus on a de chance d'être crédule selon le modèle), et le fait d'être comparativement moins éduqué que les autres. Enfin, les auteurs ont pu déterminer que les news pro-Trump étaient davantage crues par les républicains et inversement les news pro-Clinton l'étaient davantage par les démocrates. L'hypothèse des auteurs est donc confirmée : les individus ont tendance à être davantage crédules lorsque les informations vont dans le sens de leurs convictions politiques. Attention, ce résultat porte sur l'ensemble des titres (vrais et faux) et pas sur les fake news ! Le modèle ne dit pas si ce résultat s'explique parce que les individus croient davantage aux vraies informations ou parce qu'ils sont davantage crédules face aux fausses informations (ou les deux).

Conclusion

This article had a huge influence on fake news literature. It was one of the first to research the causes and to feed reflection with empirical data. It highlights the economic dimension of fake news, which has now been well documented. It also established that fake news in 2016 was mostly pro-Trump, and therefore anti-Clinton. Other articles - for which we have also created detailed summaries - aim to explain why certain individuals are more likely to believe fake news.

Topic :  Disinformation  
/
/
Edition :  Journal of Economic Perpectives  
/
Country :  United States 
/
Language  :  English 
/
Share the article
Suivre les actualités de la désinformation
S'inscrire à la newsletter
Soutenir nos actions
faire un don
Fonds de dotation pour la création de la Fondation Descartes
8, Avenue du Président Wilson 75116 Paris.
Plus d'informations
Mentions légales
Gestion des cookies
Contactez-nous
Copyright © 2024 - Site réalisé par Monsieurcom
crosschevron-downchevron-right linkedin facebook pinterest youtube rss twitter instagram facebook-blank rss-blank linkedin-blank pinterest youtube twitter instagram